Feature 28. Marking of arguments and alignment

found in question(s): 63a, 63b, 63c, 64a, 64b, 65a, 65b, 66

Definition and illustration

This feature addresses different argument marking strategies as discussed in the typological literature (e.g., Dixon 1994; Croft 2022, among others).

Question 63 addresses the type of marking (using the traditional terminology of head- and dependent-marking established by Nichols 1986). This is checked for intransitive (question 63a), transitive (question 63b), and ditransitive clauses (question 63c).

Question 64 addresses the alignment (e.g., accusative, ergative, neutral), distinguishing between nominal (question 64a) and pronominal arguments (question 64b). This distinction is necessary as there are languages where case marking is restricted to pronominal forms, e.g. English or Bwamu (see below). In languages with accusative alignment, often the O argument is marked, whereas the S or A argument remains unmarked. However, in some languages like Oromo (marked-nominative languages) have an overt marker for the S and A argument instead (see the references in Hölzl 2025b).

Question 65 focuses on syntactic alignment in the combination of intransitive and transitive clauses (making use of the term "pivot" as proposed by Dixon 1994).

Question 66 asks about the presence of differential object marking (or other types of argument marking splits). If a language exhibits differential object marking (or other types of splits), one of the two marking strategies usually allows a different range of word order possibilities (also see Seržant et al. preprint on so-called positional differential argument marking).

These questions can be illustrated with data provided by Mouwéré Bognana on the Gur language Bwamu. Bwamu makes use of dependent-marking with case (on pronouns) as well as of adpositions (collectively referred to as flagging, Haspelmath 2019). There is no head-marking (e.g., in person or number). For nominal arguments, the alignment of intransitive and transitive clauses is neutral with all three arguments being equally unmarked (S=A=O). The alignment of transitive and ditransitive clauses shows neutral (T=O=G) or indirective alignment with only the G argument being optionally marked with a dative postposition ji (T=O;G).

(1) Bwamu

[wo ɓa] vɛkɛ [le joni wã].

the.sg.ah man walk.ipfv the.sg.tp stairs on

'The man walks (up the stairs).'

(2) Bwamu

[wo ɓa] [boẽzɛ].

the.sg.ah man buy dog

'The man buys a dog.'

(3) Bwamu

[wo ɓa] ni [boẽzɛ] [wo wizɛ ji].

the.sg.ah man give dog the.sg.ah boy to

'The man gives a dog to the boy'

For pronouns, the alignment is different as there are suppletive accusative forms. For instance, the third person form wo is used for the S and A function.

(4) Bwamu

wo vɛkɛ [le joni wã].

3sg walk.ipfv the.sg.tp stairs on

'He walks (up the stairs).'

However, special accusative forms are employed for the O argument. In the first person, this accusative form is mi instead of ĩ.

(5) Bwamu

wo bi wa mi.

3sg not love 1sg.acc

'He hates me.'

(6) Bwamu

ĩ bi wa a.

1sg not love 3sg.acc

'I hate him.'

The second person has fo instead of ũ and the third person a instead of wo.

(7) Bwamu

wo bi wa fo.

3sg not love 2sg

'He hates you.'

(8) Bwamu

ũ bi wa a.

2sg not love 3sg.acc

'You hate him.'

Thus, there is accusative alignment within the pronominal system (S=A;O). Given the distinction between unmarked (nomimal) and marked (pronominal) objects, there is also differential object marking.

Coordinating an intransitive and a transitive clause that share the subject is possible. In the following example, the shared argument wo ɓa 'the man' is the S argument in the first and the A argument in the second clause. Since it can be left out in the second clause, the language exhibits an S/A pivot.

(9) Bwamu

[wo ɓa]S buakɛ ho lo

the.sg.ah man walk.around the.sg.tp corner and

([wo ɓa]A) mi [musũ]O.

the.sg.ah man see cat

'The man walks around the corner and the man sees a cat'

But Bwamu has no S/O pivot (or syntactic ergativity) as the argument wo ɓa 'the man' cannot act as S argument in the first and O argument in the second clause.

(10) Bwamu

*[wo ɓa]S buakɛ ho lo

the.sg.ah man walk.around the.sg.tp corner and

[musũ]A mi ([wo ɓa]O).

cat see the.sg.ah man

Intended: 'The man walks around the corner and a cat sees the man'

Correlations

There is a (diachronic) connection of OV order and the presence of case marking (Shcherbakova et al. 2024; Hölzl preprint).

Languages with SVO order tend to have zero marking instead (Sinnemäki 2010).

Ergativity seems to be more common in languages with OV order (see the references in Hölzl 2025a).

Given the possible connection of ergativity with OV order, it could be the case that S/O pivots are also more common in languages with OV order (cf. Dixon 1994).

References

Author(s)TitleYearPublished in
Seržant, Ilja, Sergey Say, Andreas Hölzl, Aigul Zakirova, Gao Xinyi & Andreas PreglaA typology of positional differential argument marking. preprintLinguistic Typology. 1-31.
Sinnemäki, KaiusWord order in zero-marking languages.2010Studies in Language 34: 869-912.
Hölzl, AndreasPostnominal flagging and OV in Sinitic: Areal and typological perspectives.preprintStudies in Language 49.
Croft, WilliamMorphosyntax: Constructions of the world's languages.2022Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dixon, Robert M. W.Ergativity.1994Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Haspelmath, MartinIndexing and flagging, and head and dependent marking.2019Te Reo 62(1). 93–115.
Hölzl, AndreasErgativsprache [ergative language].2025aIn Mechthild Habermann & Ilse Wischer (eds.), Historische Sprachwissenschaft (Wörterbücher zur Sprach- und Kommunikationswissenschaft (WSK) Online). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Hölzl, AndreasNominativsprache [nominative language].2025bIn Mechthild Habermann & Ilse Wischer (eds.), Historische Sprachwissenschaft (Wörterbücher zur Sprach- und Kommunikationswissenschaft (WSK) Online). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Nichols, JohannaHead-marking and dependent-marking grammar.1986Language 66(1). 56–119.
Shcherbakova, Olena, Damián E. Blasi, Volker Gast, Hedvig Skirgård, Russell D. Gray & Simon J. GreenhillThe evolutionary dynamics of how languages signal who does what to whom.2024Scientific Reports 14. 7259.